MARCIA DAY CHILDRESS: Good afternoon. 00:29 I'd like to welcome you to this Medical Center Hour. 00:32 This is a program entitled, Is There Life After Death? 00:36 Fifty Years of Research at UVA. 00:40 I'm Marcia Day Childress from the Center for Biomedical 00:43 Ethics and Humanities, and we're delighted to produce 00:46 the Medical Center Hour and bring it to you each week 00:50 during the academic semesters. 00:54 I would quickly ask that there are a few-- 00:57 a couple of seats still left. 00:59 Anybody sitting on the stairs, I would 01:02 encourage you please to find a seat, 01:04 because the Fire Marshall who sometimes 01:06 visits us doesn't approve of people sitting on the stairs. 01:13 Does some aspect of our personality 01:15 survive bodily death? 01:18 Long a philosophical and theological question. 01:21 In the 20th century, this became the subject 01:24 of scientific research. 01:27 50 Years ago this year in 1967, Dr. Ian Stevenson 01:32 then Chair of UVA's Department of Psychiatry 01:36 within the department a research unit now known 01:40 as the Division of Perceptual Studies 01:43 to study what, if anything, of the human personality 01:46 survives after death. 01:48 Dr. Stevenson's own research investigated 01:51 hundreds of accounts of young children 01:54 who claimed to recall past lives. 01:58 In our Medical Center Hour today, 01:59 one of our History of the Health Sciences 02:01 lectures, faculty from the Division of Perceptual Studies 02:06 highlights this unit's research initiatives since its founding. 02:11 They will also preview the unit priorities and partnerships 02:14 for its second half century. 02:17 We are delighted to welcome four presenters 02:20 to cover this span of studies. 02:24 On my immediate right, Jim B. Tucker, 02:27 who is the Bonner-Lowry Associate Professor 02:29 of Psychiatry and Neuro-behavioral Sciences. 02:34 He's also the director of the Division. 02:38 Next, is Bruce Greyson, Chester Carlson Professor Emeritus 02:43 of Psychiatry and Neuro-behavioral Sciences. 02:46 Third is Kim Penberthy, the Chester Carlson Professor 02:50 of Psychiatry and Neuro-behavioral Sciences, 02:53 and finally, on my far right Ed Kelly, 02:56 Research Professor of Psychiatry and Neuro-behavioral Sciences. 03:00 They will be presenting in this order. 03:04 By way quickly of some historical background, 03:07 I'll mention that the Medical Center Hour in past years 03:11 often addressed the Division of Perceptual Studies 03:14 research, especially Ian Stevenson's internationally 03:18 known studies of children with possible past lives. 03:22 And indeed, Ian presented here multiple times about his work. 03:27 His research, like that of his successors in the Division, 03:30 is I think especially noteworthy for scientific rigor 03:35 with which it was performed. 03:37 Indeed, Dr. Stevenson is standard for impeccable science 03:41 was extraordinarily high, and it even continuously challenged 03:45 him in his own work. 03:48 I'd like to say that today's program is 03:51 co-presented with Historical Collections of the Health 03:54 Sciences Library and with the Department of Psychiatry 03:57 and Neuro-behavioral Sciences here at UVA. 04:01 All of the speakers have completed 04:03 disclosure forms and none had conflicts 04:05 of interest to disclose. 04:07 So we'll start with Dr. Tucker. 04:09 Welcome. 04:09 [APPLAUSE] 04:17 JIM TUCKER: Thank you very much. 04:19 As you have heard, we are going to try 04:21 to cover a fair amount today. 04:23 I'm going to start by telling a little but about the history 04:26 of our Division, and then we'll each talk 04:29 about one particular area of research. 04:32 And mine will be this work that Marcia mentioned 04:35 that Dr. Stevenson began with young children who 04:38 report memories of past lives. 04:42 As she said, we do not have anything 04:44 to disclose as far as conflicts of interest. 04:50 So the story begins with Ian Stevenson, 04:53 and he came here to be the Chairman of the Department 04:56 of Psychiatry in 1957. 05:00 At that point, he was in the middle of quite a successful 05:03 mainstream career. 05:04 He had nearly 70 publications to his credit at that point. 05:08 But he also had an interest in para-psychology. 05:12 And after he had been here a while, 05:14 he heard about these cases of children 05:18 from various parts of the world who described memories 05:21 of a past life, and he decided to go investigate those cases. 05:26 He was able to fund that travel with the help of Chester 05:31 Carlson, which is a name you've already 05:34 with people on the Chester Carlson Chair. 05:37 Chester Carlson invented the Xerox machine. 05:41 So he was quite wealthy, and he became a big supporter 05:44 of Ian's. 05:46 So in 1967 with Carlson's help, Ian 05:51 was able to step down as chairman in the department 05:54 and establish the-- what we now call the Division of Perceptual 05:58 Studies. 06:01 So this year marks our 50 year anniversary, 06:05 and we've been going strong ever since then. 06:09 And most of the time we were-- our home 06:13 was this old clapboard house on Wertland Street, 06:17 which is now student housing. 06:21 A few years ago we moved to our current home, which 06:24 is near the downtown mall, and we certainly 06:27 don't occupy the entire building. 06:30 But we do have the Ian Stevenson Memorial Library there, which 06:34 has over 5,000 books in it. 06:36 And we also have a neuro-imaging lab. 06:41 And as for Ian, once he established the Division 06:45 in 1967, he spent the bulk of the next 40 years 06:51 focused on these cases of children 06:53 reporting past life memories. 06:55 He published numerous books and papers about them. 07:00 One of his books was reviewed in JAMA, 07:05 and it's actually reviewed by the book review editor who 07:08 wrote, "in regard to reincarnation, 07:12 he has painstakingly and unemotionally 07:14 collected a detailed series of cases from India. 07:18 Cases in which the evidence is difficult to explain 07:21 on any other grounds. 07:23 He's placed on record a large amount of data that cannot be 07:27 ignored." 07:29 So to tell you a little bit about his phenomenon, 07:32 we had now studied over 2,500 cases around the world. 07:38 They are easiest to find in cultures 07:40 with a belief in reincarnation. 07:42 But they have been found wherever 07:44 anyone has looked for them. 07:46 They have been found on all the continents, except Antarctica. 07:51 And they typically involve very young children 07:54 who spontaneously start talking about a past life, 07:59 and a recent ordinary past life. 08:01 These kids are not claiming to be Cleopatra or Julius Caesar 08:05 or anything like that. 08:07 Just describing somebody who lived and died. 08:11 The one part of the life that's often out of the ordinary 08:14 is how the previous person died. 08:17 In over 70% of the cases, the previous person 08:21 died by unnatural means meaning murder, suicide, accident, 08:26 combat, that sort of thing. 08:30 And often the kids in most cases that we've investigated, 08:35 the kids give enough details so that people 08:39 have been able to confirm that somebody did actually live 08:42 and die whose life matches the details that the child gave. 08:48 Now along with the statements, that children often 08:51 shows emotional or behavioral features that seem connected 08:55 to the material they are describing, 08:57 and I'll give you an example of that in a minute. 09:01 In addition, some children even had birth marks or birth 09:05 defects that match wounds, usually 09:08 the fatal wounds on the body of the previous person. 09:12 And Ian studied a lot of these cases. 09:15 One was a little girl who remembered 09:17 the life of a man who got his fingers chopped off 09:19 as he was being murdered, and the little girl 09:22 was born with her hands looking like that. 09:26 There was a boy who remembered the life of a boy 09:29 in another village who had lost the fingers of his right hand 09:33 in a fodder chopping machine, and the second little boy 09:37 was born with his hands looking like that, which 09:40 is quite an unusual defect. 09:44 And then there was a boy who remembered 09:46 the life of the man who had been killed by a shotgun blast 09:49 to the side of his head, and the little boy was born 09:53 was born with just a stump for an ear and underdeveloped right 09:57 side of his face. 10:00 Ian also listed 18 cases in which children 10:03 were born with two birthmarks, ones 10:06 that matched both the entrance wound and the exit 10:08 wound on the body of a gunshot victim. 10:13 Now in recent years, we have focused more on American cases. 10:18 And we can now say with certainty 10:21 that this is not purely a cultural phenomenon that 10:24 takes place in areas with belief of reincarnation, 10:28 because we know lots of American cases. 10:30 And most of them take place in families 10:34 who have never believed in reincarnation 10:36 before the children started talking about a past life. 10:41 So I want to give you an example of a case, 10:45 and then I'll turn things over to Bruce. 10:47 This is a case that got some publicity a few years ago. 10:51 It's a little boy named James Leininger. 10:54 Well, he's not so little anymore. 10:56 But James was the son of a Christian couple in Louisiana, 11:02 and his dad, in particular, was quite opposed 11:05 to the idea of reincarnation before all this started. 11:09 But around the time of his second birthday, 11:12 James started having horrible nightmares multiple times 11:16 a week in which he would be kicking his legs up in the air 11:19 and screaming airplane crash on fire, little man can't get out. 11:25 And during the day, he would take his little toy airplanes, 11:28 and he would say "Airplane crash on fire," 11:31 and bam, he would slam them in the family's coffee table. 11:35 He did this over and over again and his parents are apparently 11:39 tolerant people, because their coffee table had 11:45 dozens of scratches and dents from "Airplane crash on fire," 11:49 bam. 11:51 So when you add that play to the nightmares 11:56 that he was having, he really looked like a traumatized 11:59 child. 12:00 But he had not experienced any trauma, at least in this life. 12:05 And then a little while after his second birthday, 12:09 his parents were able to have several conversations with him 12:13 during the day in which he could talk about those things. 12:16 And he said how his plane had crashed on fire, 12:19 and how he had been shot down by the Japanese. 12:24 And he said that he flew a Corsair. 12:26 Now I'd never heard of Corsair, but it 12:29 was a special plane that was developed during World War II. 12:34 Then when he was 28 months old, he 12:36 said one day that his plane had flown off of a boat. 12:40 And his parents asked him the name of the boat, 12:43 and he said Natoma, and it turns out 12:47 there was a USS Natoma Bay that was stationed in the Pacific 12:51 during World War II. 12:54 Then when he was 2 and 1/2 his father 12:58 bought this book on Iwo Jima to give his own father, James' 13:02 grandfather for Christmas. 13:05 And he was looking through it one day when James came and got 13:07 in his lap. 13:09 And they were thumbing through it, and they got to this page. 13:13 And James' pointed at the picture and said "That's where 13:17 my plane was shot down." 13:19 And his dad said, "What?" 13:20 And he said, "My airplane got shot down there daddy." 13:24 And that just floored his dad that his 2 and 1/2 year old 13:27 was talking like that. 13:30 And then he learned that, in fact, 13:32 that Natoma Bay did take part in the Iwo Jima operation. 13:38 Then James got old enough to draw, 13:41 he drew dozens and dozens of pictures of planes and battle 13:45 scenes, and he always signed them James III 13:48 and said that he was the third James. 13:53 Well, eventually, with all this going on, 13:55 his dad begin to wonder if he was remembering a past life. 13:59 So when James was 4 1/2, he went to a Natoma Bay reunion. 14:05 And he learned that one, and only one, pilot from the ship 14:09 had been killed during the Iwo Jima operation. 14:13 This was a young man from Pennsylvania 14:16 named James Huston. 14:18 So what we can do is compare was James Leininger 14:23 said to James Huston's life to see how well that they match. 14:27 Now James' parents said that he also talked about family life 14:31 before the war, but we don't have documentation 14:35 of those statements that was made 14:36 before Huston was identified. 14:39 But what we do have here, this is a list 14:41 of items where we do have definite documentation named 14:46 before anyone knew anything about James Huston. 14:51 So James signed his drawings "James three." 14:55 Huston was James Jr., which would make James 14:58 Leininger the third James. 15:01 James said they flew off the Natoma. 15:04 Huston was the pilot on the USS Natoma Bay. 15:09 James said they flew a Corsair. 15:11 Huston had flown a Corsair. 15:12 He was actually flying a different plane 15:14 when he was killed, but he was part 15:16 of the squadron that tested the Corsair for the Navy. 15:19 James said he was shot down by the Japanese, 15:22 and Huston was shot down by the Japanese. 15:25 James said, he died in Iwo Jima. 15:28 Huston was the one and only Natoma Bay pilot killed 15:31 during the Iwo Jima operation. 15:34 James said one day quote, "my airplane got shot in the engine 15:38 and crashed into the water, and that's how I died." 15:43 Eye witnesses reported that Huston's plane was quote, 15:46 "hit head on right on the middle of the engine." 15:50 James had nightmares of his plane crashing and sinking 15:53 in the water, and Huston's plane crashed in the water 15:57 and quickly sank. 15:59 And James said one day that his friend, Jack Larson, was there. 16:03 And Jack Larson was the pilot of the plane 16:05 next to Huston's on the day that he was killed. 16:09 James is now 18 years old. 16:12 He graduated from high school last spring, 16:15 and he has now joined the Navy. 16:18 So with that, I will turn things over to Bruce Greyson. 16:21 [APPLAUSE] 16:32 BRUCE GREYSON: Thank you, Jim. 16:34 A lot of the phenomena that we've 16:35 been studying at the Division of Perceptual Studies 16:38 or DOPS for the past 40 years have been new death experiences 16:43 or NDEs. 16:45 Many people when they come close to death 16:49 report unusual and profound experiences 16:51 in which they appear to have left their bodies 16:54 and claim to have moved beyond the boundaries of time 16:57 and space. 16:59 These NDEs have been recorded in a variety of ancient cultures. 17:03 You can find them in the writings of Cleo, in the Bible, 17:06 in writings from Tibet, India, Egypt, China, Japan, 17:11 and in the folklore of the South Pacific and Native Americans. 17:15 The interpretation of these experiences 17:17 varies from culture to culture, but the basic phenomena 17:20 remain the same across the globe and across the centuries. 17:25 These experiences near death were 17:26 recorded in medical literature and journals 17:30 in the 19th century. 17:32 And they were described as a discrete syndrome in 1892 17:35 by Heim. 17:38 They were written about quite a bit in fresh academic journals 17:41 in the 1890s when Victor Egger gave the term near death 17:45 experiences. 17:47 That term became popular in English 17:49 in 1975 when the book was written 17:52 by a UVA intern named Raymond Moody called Life After Life. 17:57 There remained some controversy now about what causes NDEs, 18:01 and what there ultimate meaning is, 18:02 but there's no question about there incidence 18:04 or their impact on people. 18:07 A variety of studies here at UVA and in Europe and the UK 18:12 suggests that they occur in about 20% 18:14 of people who have a documented cardiac arrest in study 18:18 after study. 18:20 We at UVA have developed a model of the NDE and a scale 18:24 to quantify the depth of NDEs that's 18:27 been used in hundreds of studies around the world. 18:31 It basically looks at NDEs as being composed 18:33 of component parts, changes in thought processes, 18:37 changes in emotional states, apparently 18:40 paranormal phenomena, and what seem 18:42 to be otherworldly phenomena. 18:46 The changes in thought processes include a sense 18:48 of time being distorted. 18:50 People often talk about having a sense of timelessness, 18:55 thinking, being faster, and clearer than usual, 18:58 having a life review or panoramic memory where 19:00 your entire life flashes before your eyes, 19:03 and a sense of sudden understanding or revelation 19:05 where everything becomes crystal clear. 19:09 The changes in feeling include a sense of peace and well-being, 19:13 feelings of joy, a sense of oneness 19:15 or cosmic unity, an encounter with what 19:19 seems to be a loving, warm being of light. 19:22 The apparently paranormal features 19:23 include extraordinary sensory vividness. 19:27 People report seeing colors they have never seen on Earth, 19:29 hearing sounds they've never heard before, 19:32 having what seems to be frank extrasensory 19:34 perception of things going on elsewhere, 19:37 visions of the future, and a sense of leaving 19:39 the physical body. 19:42 The otherworldly features include 19:43 being in an otherworldly realm, an unearthly or mystical realm. 19:49 Encountering some mystical being or presence, 19:51 seeing deceased spirits or religious spirits, 19:55 and coming to a point of no return beyond which you 19:57 can't come back. 19:59 Now most NDEs, in fact, have a combination of all four 20:02 of these elements to varying degrees. 20:05 As an example, let me give you a report of one woman who 20:09 had the following experience. 20:11 She said, during the war I was very ill in the hospital. 20:16 One morning the nurse came in and found me showing 20:19 no sign of life whatever. 20:21 She called the doctors to whom I also appeared dead, 20:25 and I remained so they told me afterwards 20:27 for at least 20 minutes. 20:30 I became aware of a brilliant light and felt drawn toward it. 20:34 It seemed that time was different or nonexistent there, 20:36 wherever there was. 20:38 The light was beautiful to look at 20:41 and projected feelings of unconditional love and peace. 20:45 Looking around, I found myself in a beautiful green, 20:48 undulating country. 20:51 I then saw a young officer with a few soldiers approaching. 20:55 The office was my favorite cousin, Albin. 20:59 I did not know that he had died, nor had I 21:02 ever seen him in uniform. 21:04 But what I saw of him was confirmed by a photograph 21:07 I had seen many years later. 21:10 We spoke for a few minutes happily, 21:12 and then he and the few men with him marched off. 21:16 Then a presence beside me explaining 21:18 that these soldiers were allowed to go and greet 21:20 others who were dying and help them meet their death. 21:24 My next vivid recollection after this 21:27 was of looking down from about ceiling height 21:31 onto a bed on which laid a very emaciated body. 21:35 There were doctors and nurses around it. 21:38 I yelled out to them, but they couldn't hear me. 21:41 I could see everything clearly and felt warm, safe, 21:44 and peaceful. 21:47 In a few moments, I was looking up at them 21:49 and feeling sensation of intense disappointment. 21:53 I had come back from something so lovely and so utterly 21:55 satisfying. 21:57 Two days later, the doctor told me I was lucky I didn't die. 22:01 I told him that I did. 22:05 He looked at me in a strange way and scheduled me 22:09 for psychological evaluation. 22:12 I learned to keep my mouth shut about that from that time on. 22:17 Now one of the problems we have in researching 22:19 near death experiences is that for the most part, 22:22 they're retrospective. 22:23 We're getting accounts from people 22:25 who had the experiences sometime in the past. 22:28 That leaves open the question of how reliable memories of NDEs 22:31 really are. 22:33 Some authors have speculated that memories 22:36 are embellished over the years. 22:38 In particularly, that the sense of well-being and peace 22:41 and the pleasantness of the experience 22:43 gets embellished over time. 22:45 Because we've been studying these experiences for four 22:47 decades now, we're able to address this question. 22:51 Starting in 2002, I started trying 22:54 to reconnect with people I had interviewed 22:55 in the 1980s about their near death experiences 22:59 and asked them to describe their NDEs for me again. 23:03 What we found-- oops, I'm going the wrong way here-- 23:08 is that the NDE scale scores measuring the depth of the NDE 23:12 were the same now in the 2000s as they were in the 1980s. 23:20 And that held true for all four of the components, changes 23:23 in thinking, changes in feeling, paranormal, and transcendental. 23:27 So memories of the NDE are, indeed, reliable over yea. 23:31 And that's in just that retrospect to research 23:33 is also reliable. 23:36 Another important question about retrospective reports of NDEs 23:39 is whether they're influenced by cultural beliefs. 23:43 We know that people's cultural beliefs influence how they 23:46 interpret their perceptions. 23:48 We see what we expect. 23:50 For example, near death experiences 23:52 of third world countries do not talk 23:54 about entering a tunnel the way Americans do. 23:56 They would talk about entering a cave or a well. 23:59 One truck driver who I interviewed 24:01 talked about entering a tailpipe. 24:04 So you have to use whatever cultural metaphors 24:06 at your disposal to describe the phenomenon. 24:10 So our NDErs just reporting what they 24:11 expect to happen when they come close to death. 24:15 The image of NDEs that most people have nowadays 24:18 is the one described by Raymond Moody in 1975. 24:22 Now we've been collecting NDEs here at UVA since 1960s, 24:27 years before Moody's book came out. 24:30 So we compared 24 experiences we collected in the 1960s 24:34 with 24 recent experiences that were matched 24:38 with the original ones in terms of age, race, gender, religion, 24:42 cause of death and proximity to death. 24:45 What we found is that the features that Moody reported, 24:49 were reported just as often before the experience 24:52 and experience. 24:54 No matter what we looked at, the out-of-body experience, 24:57 the feeling of peace, meeting others, a being of light, 25:01 noises, or life review all reported before we 25:04 Moody had described them as often as they are now. 25:08 And they also hold true for the after events 25:10 that Moody reported, attitude changes, loss of fear of death, 25:15 difficulty telling others, belief in survival 25:18 after afterlife, and corroboration 25:20 of extrasensory perceptions, or as often 25:23 before described them as after. 25:26 So report do not seem to be influenced 25:29 by the widespread public knowledge of NDEs. 25:32 Although the interpretation of the phenomenon 25:35 maybe influence by culture, the actual experience 25:37 appears not to be. 25:40 But even though these memories of NDEs 25:42 are reliable and consistent over decades, 25:46 that doesn't establish that their memories are 25:48 real events rather than memories of fantasies or hallucinations. 25:54 To test that possibility, we use the memory characteristics 25:57 questionnaire, which was designed 25:59 to differentiate memories of real events 26:01 from memories of imagined events. 26:04 This memory characteristics questionnaire taps 26:07 into the five aspects of memories 26:09 that reliably differentiate memories 26:11 of real from imagined events. 26:13 It includes the clarity of the memories including 26:16 the visual detail, sensory aspects, like sound, smell, 26:20 taste, in the memory, contextual features, 26:22 like the memory for location and spatial arrangements, 26:26 thoughts and feelings during the recalled event, 26:28 and the intensity of feelings both during the event 26:31 and now remembering it. 26:34 We asked people who had come close to death 26:36 to rate their memories of that event, and also of real events 26:40 that happened around the same time in their lives. 26:43 And also about an imagined event from that time in their lives. 26:48 What we found is that for those people who had NDEs, 26:51 the near death experience was remembered 26:53 with more clarity, more detail, more context, and more intense 26:57 feelings that real events from the same time period. 27:03 NDEs were recalled as realer than real events 27:06 to the same degree that real events remembered as realer 27:09 than the imaginary events. 27:13 On the other hand, people who did not have NDEs 27:15 reported there close brush with death 27:17 to be as well as other real events, but not realer. 27:23 So NDEs are remembered with great consistency over decades, 27:27 and they're recalled as realer then real. 27:30 How do we explain them? 27:32 There are no variables that we found yet 27:34 that can predict whether someone's going to have an NDE. 27:37 Neither age, race, gender, religion, religiosity, 27:42 or mental illness. 27:44 There's been lots of speculation about physiological variables 27:47 that may be involved in NDEs, but the bottom line 27:50 is that it's hard to reconcile enhanced mental function, 27:54 thinking and perception being clearer and sharper than ever 27:56 before with the impaired brain function 27:59 as you have in deep anesthesia or cardiac arrest. 28:04 So why are near death experiences of interest 28:06 to health professionals? 28:08 One reason is that there's a consistent pattern 28:11 of after effects from near death experiences. 28:13 Changes in beliefs, attitudes, and guidance, 28:16 and it's even corroborated with long term studies 28:19 and interviews with significant others. 28:22 We see increases in spirituality, 28:24 a sense of concern or compassion for others, 28:27 appreciation of life, a sense of meaning or purpose, 28:30 confidence and flexibility in your coping skills, 28:33 and a belief in postmortem survival. 28:36 And along with these we see decreases 28:37 in fear of death, the decreased interest in material 28:40 possessions, decreased interest in status, power, prestige, 28:44 and fame, and decreased interest in competition. 28:49 A second reason that NDEs are important to us 28:52 is what they suggest about survival of bodily death. 28:56 As Dr. Tucker mentioned, our Division 28:58 was founded to explore the possibility that something 29:01 may survive after death, and NDEs do provide some evidence 29:05 on that question. 29:08 For example, we have enhanced cognition 29:12 when the brain is impaired, such as by anesthesia 29:14 or cardiac arrest, which suggests that the mind is not 29:17 just what the brain does. 29:20 Second, we have accurate perceptions 29:22 from an out-of-body perspective. 29:24 A recent review of over 100 published cases 29:26 of people who had left their bodies during an NDE 29:28 and reported seeing things, showed that greater than 90% 29:32 were 100% accurate. 29:34 Further evidence of potential independence of mind and brain. 29:39 Third, we get accurate information 29:41 that's imparted by deceased visitors in the NDE, 29:45 such as the location of an important document or a hidden 29:48 treasure, which is evidence that these were encounters 29:51 with interactive beings not just mental images of the deceased. 29:54 A shocking example of this are NDEs in which the person meets 29:59 in the experience someone who was not known at the time 30:02 to be dead. 30:04 An example with the experiencer I mentioned before, 30:07 who saw her cousin, Albin, who she had known had died. 30:12 Of course, she did know that he was a soldier, so it's 30:16 conceivable she could have imagined that he had died. 30:19 But that's not always the case. 30:21 We also have the NDE from a young girl, an only child who 30:25 almost died during heart surgery and said 30:28 that in her near-death experience during surgery, 30:31 she needs someone who identified himself as her brother. 30:35 When she told her father about this 30:37 he was so moved that he confessed to her 30:39 that he had had a son she didn't know about who 30:41 had died before she was born. 30:45 We have identified dozens of cases of this type. 30:48 Some going back to ancient Greece. 30:51 The bottom line is that our culture 30:53 tends to talk about death as if it's the end. 30:57 The end of all existence. 30:58 But NDE suggests that it may be more like a change of state. 31:08 I'm going to turn things over now to Dr. Kim Penberthy. 31:11 [APPLAUSE] 31:18 I'm happy to follow the snow men. 31:23 Well, thank you. 31:25 I am Kim Penberthy, and I am the newest member at DOPS, 31:28 and I feel very honored to be part of this esteemed group 31:33 bring with me what I'm going to talk 31:36 about now, which is some of the directions we're moving in. 31:39 As you heard, there's been an extensive history 31:42 of research in the area of near-death experiences 31:45 in children who remember past lives. 31:49 That's not all that we are doing, though, or have done. 31:52 And so I'm going to describe to you a little bit, 31:55 very briefly some of the work I'm doing, 31:58 and then my colleague, Dr. Kelly, will finish up. 32:02 So the areas you heard of are important, 32:07 because of the main question and mission that DOPS has. 32:11 This idea of survival. 32:12 This idea of the relationship between what 32:15 we call the mind and the brain, and are we more than just 32:18 the bodies that we inhabit. 32:21 Additional research this builds in that area, 32:24 which would make sense. 32:25 So the areas of research that we've strategically 32:29 outlined and planned and, sort of, begun to collaborate 32:33 include areas that build upon this, including continued work 32:37 in areas you've heard about. 32:39 We're also looking to expand and focus our research 32:42 in the area of end of life. 32:44 I work in the cancer center as a clinician. 32:46 I am a clinical psychologist working with palliative care, 32:50 and we have a lot of questions about all the experiences 32:53 of people while they were dying, the experiences of people 32:56 who were working with the dying, and we'll 33:00 hope to launch research in that area 33:02 and begin hopefully this fall with a big event that we have. 33:07 We also have a strong background in the neuroimaging field 33:13 and have specific dedicated researchers to look at this. 33:16 So the folks you've just heard about, and you've heard 33:18 alluded to, we can look at their brains 33:22 and look at what is going on with various imaging 33:26 sort of strategies, including EEG, other imaging capacities 33:32 that we have. 33:33 And we have brilliant people doing that work. 33:35 So we're always looking for individuals 33:38 where we can, sort of, use these techniques 33:41 and develop our hypotheses and test them. 33:45 We also are looking at the phenomenon that 33:49 exist where we can experience some of this ability 33:58 without having the near- death experience 34:00 or a memory of a past life. 34:04 These are some extreme examples that 34:07 promote these things that Dr.-- 34:09 that Bruce was talking about. 34:11 And one of the areas of research for me 34:15 is how can we achieve these sorts 34:19 of states of minds, this level of consciousness 34:22 with intentionality. 34:24 So not all of us will have a near-death experience. 34:27 Not all of us will remember a past life. 34:30 Does that mean we're excluded from this realm 34:32 of understanding? 34:34 And I don't believe that's necessarily the case. 34:37 So my interests fold into what we've talked about. 34:45 This idea of the-- 34:49 this optimistic idea that we can achieve this. 34:56 So one area of research that I have 34:59 is looking at intentional strategies 35:02 to develop these and understand this relationship 35:06 between consciousness and the physical world. 35:09 So I have currently got funding to look 35:13 at the development of these skills, 35:16 and how they may be associated to people 35:19 who are practicing mindfulness, meditation, 35:23 various forms of yoga. 35:25 And I'm collaborating with my colleagues 35:26 at the Institute of Noetic Sciences in California 35:30 where we have some preliminary data 35:32 to demonstrate that there seems to be an association. 35:36 And the reality is-- 35:38 I mean this is what some of these practices developed for 35:42 are to enhance these abilities. 35:44 And we often in our modern day research don't ask about them. 35:50 So we might teach someone how to meditate and measure 35:53 their blood pressure and see that their blood pressure went 35:56 down, which is pretty darn amazing in and of itself. 36:00 We don't ask them, however, did you notice 36:02 if you were more intuitive, or that you've 36:06 had any kind of other extraordinary experiences. 36:10 And if you-- it turns out if you begin to ask people, 36:13 many of them we'll report that. 36:16 So we're now currently looking at prospectively monitoring 36:19 this in cohorts here and in California, 36:23 and looking at it again in the tradition of DOPS 36:27 in a scientific way, what we can find in that area. 36:31 I have researched looking at developing 36:35 a mindfulness-based intervention for people with lupus. 36:39 There have been very few developments 36:41 in this area for decades, and we were 36:45 charged with this task of looking at can doing 36:49 a mindfulness exercise not only reduce 36:53 the stress, improved fatigue, but can it actually change 36:57 biological markers. 36:59 Markers of mental logical functioning or inflammation, 37:03 IL-6, these sorts of things. 37:05 And again this is a big deal, if we 37:07 can look at intentionally developing 37:11 these skills in order to help ourselves that way 37:14 and help other people. 37:17 So through the research at DOPS, I 37:21 would just like to emphasize what 37:22 we do is we strive to challenge some 37:24 of the entrenched mainstream views that 37:28 may be held in medicine even. 37:30 And evaluation empirical evidence regarding 37:34 consciousness, and its relationship 37:36 to the physical world, including our body and our-- 37:40 and the mind and the brain and looking 37:42 at whether they are indeed distinct and separate. 37:49 This is the work we do. 37:51 And I'm going to hand it over to my colleague 37:54 to discuss this a bit further, because it's 37:58 important to understand the ramification of what 38:00 this might mean not just for ourselves, 38:03 our health, our ability. 38:05 But I would challenge you to think 38:07 about for the future of our death and our people as a race. 38:13 So thank you. 38:14 [APPLAUSE] 38:26 So I'm to try to explain in the next five or six minutes 38:33 the implications of those two large books. 38:35 Yeah, I'll just stay here. 38:37 Obviously, it's going to have to be very telegraphic. 38:41 But we put out brochures both down here 38:43 and on the table outside where you can find us, 38:46 on your way to our website, which 38:48 contains lots more information, and the real justification 38:53 for some outrageous things that I'm now going to tell you. 38:57 My career, kind of, brackets what goes on at DOPS. 39:02 A little different background, I started out 39:04 as an experimental psychologist studying psychology 39:08 and language and cognitive science 39:09 at Harvard in the 1960s. 39:11 This is when we were recovering from the dark ages 39:13 of behaviorism. 39:15 Beginning to realize that there are things like minds 39:18 and consciousness and so on. 39:21 And I mean I had absorbed the view that most scientists held 39:26 then and most do now, which is that basically mind 39:30 and consciousness are generated by physiological processes 39:33 going on in brains. 39:35 Well late in the-- 39:37 late in the piece when I was working on my dissertation, 39:40 I became interested in experimental parapsychiology. 39:44 This had to do with some experiences 39:45 involving my sister, and I learned to my amazement 39:49 that there was a vast section of Widener Library devoted 39:52 to the subject of psychical research, 39:54 including a lot of experimental research 39:56 that I've never heard of before. 39:58 And so I began reading it, and eventually 40:01 decided well this stuff looks really interesting, 40:04 and if it really happens, something's 40:06 fundamentally wrong with our basic scientific outlook. 40:09 And so I decided to sign up with JB Ryan down at-- in Durham. 40:15 He had been at Duke most of his career, 40:17 but had moved off campus. 40:19 And so I started there at the usual $400 a month, 40:22 six month trial period, and the lots of idealistic young people 40:26 who clambered to occupy those few slots. 40:31 Within a month, I met a guy who erased 40:34 any doubts I still had about the existence 40:36 of the basic phenomena that Ryan was studying. 40:39 This guy could do basically anything we asked him to do. 40:42 He succeeded at controlled psy task 40:45 at prodigious levels of statistical significance. 40:49 So the first take home lesson is, whether we like it or not, 40:53 psy-phenomenon exist as facts of nature, 40:56 and science is going to have to come to grips with that fact 40:58 somehow. 41:01 I had also gotten interested, because of some things 41:04 we found out about this guy, and the possibility of measuring 41:07 brain waves in relation to his performance in these tasks. 41:11 And I won't go into that in any detail, 41:14 but I'm happy to say that even though it, kind of, petered out 41:18 at that time, this was in the School of Engineering, 41:20 Electrical Engineering Department where I encountered 41:24 my colleague Ross Dunseath over here. 41:26 Raise your hand. 41:27 We spotted him. 41:28 He was an undergraduate at the time. 41:30 He's been with us ever since. 41:34 Anyway, the group was there. 41:36 We made some headway, but the technology really 41:38 was not up to it. 41:39 We laid the, kind of, conceptual technical foundations, 41:42 but couldn't do many of the things we hoped to do. 41:46 We now have established at DOPS a really first class 41:50 neuroimaging facility, and can begin 41:53 to do things now that we could only 41:54 dream about back in the 1970s. 41:57 OK, but now we come to the other part closer to this. 42:02 I had to give up psycho research for a long time. 42:05 I worked in neuroscience at UNC Chapel Hill. 42:09 All the UVA enemies, right, the core. 42:15 And we did some good things there 42:17 and continued to develop our EEG techniques, and so on. 42:21 But I was anxious to find a way back into the field, 42:24 and actually married one of Ian's long time research 42:28 collaborators, Emily Williams, in 1998 42:31 and retired early and moved here in 2002 so 42:34 that we could work intensely on the first book, Irreducible Mind. 42:40 Now the way that came about was that Mike Murphy 42:43 who you may know as a co-founder of Esalen Institute 42:46 out in Big Sur, California, which is now an island 42:49 apparently, by the way due to the recent storms. 42:54 He-- I mean, he's an amazing man much like Ian. 42:56 In fact, we dedicated our book to Ian and Mike 43:01 jointly even though they never met in the flesh. 43:05 They are both, kind of, the descendants 43:08 of the original founders of psychical research 43:10 that is in the breadth of their scientific vision. 43:14 Mike was well aware of the prevailing 43:16 view in neuroscience, and if that view is correct, 43:20 there can be no survival period. 43:22 That's an inescapable fact. 43:24 If mainstream view of production of mind and consciousness 43:28 by psychological processes in the brain is correct, 43:32 there can be no survival. 43:34 That's the fundamental biological objection 43:36 to survival, and it's been elaborated ad nauseam 43:40 in a reason recent book called The Myth of an Afterlife. 43:46 Anyway we started by just reviewing evidence 43:50 for survival, which Mike was very interested in. 43:52 But we gradually realized that we 43:54 needed to undertake a much bigger project in two stages. 43:58 Irreducible Mind is the first stage 44:00 where we assembled in one place a whole lot 44:02 of empirical evidence that we think 44:04 disproves the conventional view of mind, brain relations. 44:09 We incorporated all the experimental parapsychology 44:13 and survival research by reference. 44:15 We want to make a book about science survival, 44:17 but we wanted to use that evidence, 44:20 so there's a big annotated bibliography in there. 44:23 But we then went on to describe a variety 44:25 of other well-documented empirical phenomena that 44:29 are difficult or impossible to explain 44:32 from a conventional point of view. 44:34 And I won't go through it. 44:35 They include things like extreme physiological influence. 44:39 The manufacturing of things like stigmata or hypnotic blisters. 44:43 Even blisters having a specific geometrical form. 44:48 They include, in particular, cases of this sort 44:50 that Bruce talked about. 44:53 Near-death experiences under conditions 44:55 such as deep general anesthesia and or cardiac arrest. 44:59 And those are crucial, because these things 45:02 are happening under conditions that 99% of contemporary neuro 45:07 scientists believe are-- 45:11 the conditions which neuro scientists 45:13 believe are necessary for conscious experience 45:15 have been abolished. 45:18 And that can be shown categorically. 45:20 The way we know that they had the experiences during the time 45:24 of unconsciousness is that they can sometimes 45:26 report things correctly, things that happen during that period. 45:30 And there are several arguments about all that, 45:32 but that's the basic idea. 45:34 Anyway, the end result of this whole exercise, 45:37 and I have to finish within the next minute or two 45:41 was to show that the correlations that 45:45 exist between mental events and physical events, which we all 45:49 accept that's just how it is, can be 45:52 interpreted in a different way. 45:54 That it is that mind, consciousness, or something 45:58 inherently larger than we normally know that overflow 46:02 the organism in some sense would operate the condition 46:06 by the behavior of the brain and the sense organs 46:08 and all the rest of it. 46:10 Now to me that totally changed the landscape 46:13 of the conceptual territory of this survival discussion, 46:17 because now we have a way of thinking 46:20 about the mind, brain connection that allows for the possibility 46:23 that mind can operate separately from the organism. 46:27 To me that eroded the biological obstacle to survive. 46:33 OK, well now we've got that far, and so 46:35 in psychological way, what does it all mean in terms 46:37 of our fundamental worldview. 46:39 That was a second much harder job, 46:44 and we struggled with it for a long time, 46:46 but finally put together a book that's remarkable 46:50 at least for its authorship, because we have physicists, 46:54 scholars of religion, and philosophers 46:56 all under the covers of one book. 46:59 And to cut to the bottom line, the basic picture 47:01 is one that seems to convert the currently popular view 47:07 of the relation between the physical and the mental. 47:11 We have begun to think we're being driven 47:14 towards some kind of an idealistic picture in which 47:18 mind is not derivative from matter, 47:21 but if anything the other way around. 47:23 So it has fundamental implications 47:25 for our basic vision of reality and our place in it. 47:30 So I think I'll stop there, and we start questions. 47:32 [APPLAUSE] 47:40 MARCIA DAY CHILDRESS: Thank you. 47:41 Thank you for four wonderful presentations. 47:45 I daresay this is our mind expanding. 47:50 And we are open to your comments and questions. 47:54 We have a couple of mics we can bring to those of you who 47:58 have questions. 47:59 I would ask that you please identify yourself 48:01 before you make your comment or ask your question. 48:05 And you may direct your question to any or all of the presents. 48:10 So who would like to start? 48:17 AUDIENCE: I'm Bob Reynolds, a retired professor of medicine 48:20 here and used to be a vice president. 48:22 And I find this material absolutely fascinating 48:25 and mind boggling. 48:27 I'm reminded of a book made into a recent movie called 48:31 Heaven Is For Real. 48:33 And it tells the story of a three 48:35 or four-year-old boy who has a ruptured appendix, 48:38 and I presume had a near-death experience. 48:40 And as he gets older, he tells the family 48:42 about what he experienced. 48:44 Would you categorize this as one of your near-death experiences, 48:48 and the story that came with it? 48:52 BRUCE GREYSON: Well, I would certainly characterize it 48:54 as a near-death experience. 48:56 There are a lot of questions about this particular case. 48:59 He was a four-year-old boy as I said-- as you said. 49:02 And his parents were quite religious. 49:04 In fact, the father was a pastor. 49:06 And there's a lot of suggesting that his account was, 49:08 kind of, filtered through the parents belief system. 49:13 That does not, of course, change the phenomena that he reported. 49:16 He did, in fact, encounter deceased relatives 49:20 that he didn't know, and so forth. 49:22 So there are some phenomena that classify 49:24 as his legitimate near-death experience, 49:26 although the trappings may be colored by his family's belief 49:29 system. 49:33 AUDIENCE: Hi, I'm Anita Regal. 49:35 I'm a retired nurse practitioner. 49:37 First, I want to give a shout out to Dr. Tucker's books 49:39 for anybody who hasn't read them. 49:41 They're really extraordinary, and I 49:43 think it underscores the rigor of the research. 49:45 But I have a question for Dr. Greyson. 49:47 When I was a nurse practicing in the coronary care unit, 49:51 I had a few patients that I can recall that had 49:54 or that talked about what sounded 49:55 like near-death experiences. 49:57 But there was one patients in particularly 49:59 that was so disturbing, I mean I still 50:01 remember his name and the vision, 50:03 because it was really unsettling. 50:04 It was not a pleasant experience. 50:07 It was very-- he actually saw on a rainy day, he had-- 50:11 it was during a cardiac arrest. 50:12 He talked about being up in the room being able to see things 50:15 going on that were confirmed. 50:17 But then he talked about his experience 50:19 where he actually saw his funeral. 50:21 It was a horse drawn carriage. 50:22 It was very, very unsettling to him. 50:26 So it was not that peaceful, sort of, calming effect. 50:30 Have you come across any experiences like that? 50:32 I don't know if it was a near-death experience or not, 50:35 but it was just very different from anything I've ever heard. 50:38 BRUCE GREYSON: There certainly are 50:39 a number of experiences that seem to qualify as near-death, 50:43 but they are not pleasant. 50:46 We don't know how many there are, 50:47 because it's hard to get people to talk about these things. 50:51 Most people who have studied the phenomena 50:52 say that between 1 and 10% of people 50:55 who have a near-death experience will have an unpleasant one. 50:58 Although again, it's hard to say, 51:00 because they don't talk about them. 51:02 Nancy Evans Bush, who herself had a terrifying experience, 51:05 wrote a book about it called Dancing Past 51:08 the Dark, in which she describes not only 51:11 a range of unpleasant near-death experiences, 51:14 but how to deal with them, and how 51:16 to make them into creative positive experiences. 51:20 Most mythological hero's journeys, 51:24 include travels through terrifying travails 51:28 to get to get the final enlightenment or goal. 51:31 And she, kind of, interprets in times-- in terms of that. 51:33 That some people need to go through these horrible things 51:36 to get to the point. 51:38 Because we don't have many of these experiences to look at, 51:41 we don't know what goes into making a near-death experience 51:44 pleasant or unpleasant. 51:46 For some people we know that what makes them unpleasant 51:49 is the difficulty in letting go. 51:51 And people who are very rigid personalities find themselves 51:55 out of their bodies and are terrified by the experience. 51:57 So instead of embracing the experience, 51:59 they fight against, and that itself makes it terrifying. 52:05 MARCIA DAY CHILDRESS: Hi, I'm-- 52:07 I'm sorry there's one right before you sir. 52:09 So sorry. 52:10 AUDIENCE: Hi, I'm Mary Stack, and I very much 52:13 believe in what you're talking in reading things on it. 52:16 And I remember, so Oliver Sacks, in his Hallucinations, 52:21 he's poo pooing. 52:22 What is your response to what he says. 52:28 Oliver Sacks presents the standard materialistic position 52:33 that the brain creates in the mind. 52:34 And he pretty much discounts all of phenomena that we look at 52:39 and credit. 52:41 And I don't know what his rationale was 52:43 for this discounting those, except that they don't 52:45 agree with his world view. 52:46 Interestingly, he started one of his books, Musicophilia, which 52:50 then-- with a near-death experience 52:51 of Tony Cicoria, who was an orthopedic surgeon 52:55 in Rochester, who had a near-death experience when he 52:57 was struck by lightning, and then had a total personality 53:00 change. 53:01 He started composing classical music, which he had never been 53:05 involved with music before. 53:07 Totally changed his life. 53:08 And yet Sacks interpreted this as just an artifact 53:12 of his brain being fried by the electricity. 53:18 So I know that he knows of our work, and he has read it, 53:22 and just thinks it's not worth paying attention to. 53:29 AUDIENCE: I'm Greg Patterson. 53:30 I'm the Gamma Knife Nurse Coordinator here at UVA. 53:34 Is your institute using anything to study 53:37 isochronic or binaural acoustic waveforms 53:41 to alter mental status? 53:44 Such as in a lot of the popular literature, 53:46 they're using theta waves, delta waves, 53:49 to elicit out of body experiences, 53:52 healing, changing brain chemistry, 53:55 things of that nature. 53:59 EDWARD KELLY: We're certainly interested in that subject. 54:01 One of the implications of the kind of psychological model 54:04 that I sketched is that we potentially 54:08 have access to all kinds of capabilities 54:10 that normally we can't access. 54:13 Sometimes people blunder into it, 54:14 through having a near-death encounter of some sort, some 54:19 through meditating for decades, maybe 54:23 there are all kinds of ways of accessing these things that we 54:25 haven't found out about yet. 54:26 Some kind of stimulation, whether electrical 54:30 or magnetic stimulation of the brain directly, or sound 54:34 of one sort or another. 54:35 I don't think anybody has discovered anything 54:38 like that yet that is really all that effective, 54:41 but the possibilities are definitely there. 54:45 AUDIENCE: I'm Anna Tate. 54:47 I'm one of the abdominal transplant nurse practitioners 54:49 here. 54:50 I had just a quick question. 54:52 When you're talking about these near-death experiences that 54:56 involve cardiac arrests or these anaesthesia, sedative states, 55:01 does that include studying patients 55:03 that have been in prolonged comas, or ICU sedative states 55:09 but have normal brain activity? 55:13 BRUCE GREYSON: We have not studied people 55:15 in prolonged coma, simply because we 55:19 want people who have an acute event that we can look at 55:21 before, during, and after. 55:23 Some people are. 55:24 Steven Laureys at the University of Liege in Belgium-- 55:27 who has done a lot of the important work 55:29 with people who seem to be in a persistent vegetative state, 55:33 and if you talk to them they can show 55:34 these electrical responses-- he is now 55:36 very interested in near-death experiences. 55:38 Some of his grad students are working with me 55:40 in collaborative research. 55:49 AUDIENCE: Hello. 55:49 I am Victoria. 55:50 I'm a UVA student. 55:51 I have a question. 55:52 Is there a scientific explanation 55:54 why people who experience clinical death have this-- 55:59 some parts of their brain are activated 56:01 or they have external powers? 56:07 For example, in Russia, there are 56:09 a lot of psychics who see the future, 56:13 but the majority of them, they've had other trauma, 56:15 or they had a coma. 56:18 Do scientists explain what happens in the brain 56:21 that after experiencing death, they have these powers? 56:28 KIM PENBERTHY: That's you. 56:29 [LAUGHTER] 56:29 BRUCE GREYSON: I don't think we have the answer to that yet. 56:32 We certainly have a lot of anecdotal evidence 56:34 that people claim all sorts of abilities after the NDE 56:37 that they didn't have before. 56:41 It's difficult to study these with neuroimaging, 56:44 for example, because a lot of them 56:45 can't do these things on demand. 56:47 There have been a few people to be 56:49 studied in the lab who claim to be able to do 56:52 these things on demand. 56:53 But to do most neuroimaging techniques, 56:56 you have to be physically still while you're being imaged. 56:59 And a lot of these people have trouble doing that. 57:02 But we are still looking for people 57:04 who can manifest these abilities at will, 57:07 that can do it in a way that we can do neuroimaging with. 57:12 AUDIENCE: Hi. 57:12 My name is Libby Rexrode. 57:14 I'm a nurse coordinator in the surgery department. 57:17 I'm just curious how you find your subjects. 57:21 Do they come to you, or are you made aware of these incidences, 57:30 or how do you come about finding them? 57:35 JIM TUCKER: Well, these days they find us. 57:37 They go online if their child is talking about a past life, 57:41 or if somebody has a near-death experience, 57:44 or extraordinary abilities. 57:46 Then they find Ed's lab. 57:49 It would be very hard to go out and find people, 57:51 but fortunately, it's very easy these days for them to find us. 57:56 EDWARD KELLY: If I could add to that, 57:58 though, it's the hardest part of our job, 58:00 is finding suitable people to work with in our lab. 58:04 So if anybody knows of anybody who has recently-- 58:06 [LAUGHTER] 58:07 Still send them our way, please. 58:11 AUDIENCE: Dan Grogan. 58:14 Unless I missed something, the only percentile correlation 58:18 you looked at was, or mentioned, was cardiac events and trauma, 58:24 suicide, war. 58:27 Does that aspect-- 58:30 I assume you were looking at the highest correlations. 58:33 But connecting cause of death with NDE percentages, 58:41 does that area lead you in any other direction? 58:48 Or is there some-- 58:49 what's the current theory about why those areas correlate? 58:53 And then, I'd also be interested in your view of the young man 58:58 of James III, and his personality in the Western 59:02 context, rebirth or re-incarnated folks-- 59:09 what they bring to society after that in the arc of their life. 59:14 BRUCE GREYSON: I'll take the NDE part first. 59:16 We have studied-- we have files on more than 1,000 59:19 near-death experiences. 59:21 About a quarter of those are from cardiac arrest. 59:24 Most of them are from other causes of death. 59:26 The problem with a lot of these cases 59:27 is that it's hard to know how close they were to death, 59:31 because they happened in a car accident or some place 59:33 where they are not being monitored. 59:35 We like to study cardiac arrest, because we 59:37 know exactly what's going on with them, so we can 59:39 get nice figures about that. 59:42 Because with people who were in a car accident, 59:44 we don't know what the baseline is, so we don't know 59:46 how many of them have NDEs. 59:48 So it's neater to do research with cardiac arrest, 59:51 but we study other people as well. 59:53 In fact, we have not found that the way you come close to death 59:57 has any impact on that type of near-death experience 60:01 that you're going to have. 60:03 There are some exceptions, like people 60:04 who were intoxicated at the time or under the effect of drugs 60:08 tend not to have as many NDEs, or to have shallower, quote, 60:11 NDEs than people who were mentally clear at the time. 60:16 JIM TUCKER: So as far as the question of what children 60:19 who remember past lives, what they then take with them 60:22 into their life-- 60:24 most of the children, by the time 60:26 they get to be six or seven, stop talking about this stuff, 60:29 and they just go on with their lives, as far as we can tell. 60:32 Now, some of them as adults say that it did give them more 60:36 of sort of a spiritual outlook. 60:39 But for the most part, they seem to be pretty much 60:42 like the rest of us. 60:45 They're certainly not little mystics or anything like that. 60:48 They're just kind of typical kids. 60:50 We've done psychological testing with stuff some of them. 60:52 The only thing that really showed up 60:54 is they tend to be quite intelligent. 60:56 But otherwise, they're pretty much just like everybody else. 61:00 MARCIA DAY CHILDRESS: So we've come to the end of hour, 61:03 but obviously, not to the end of your questions. 61:06 So you'd be welcome to come down and talk with the presenters 61:10 following the program. 61:12 We invite you to join us next week, March 1st, at 12:00 noon, 61:18 please, because we're combined with Medical Grand Round. 61:21 We have Gerard Clancy here with us, 61:25 who's president of the University of Tulsa 61:27 in Tulsa, Oklahoma. 61:28 He's also a psychiatrist. 61:31 He will be talking about a story of success 61:34 in reversing urban health disparities. 61:37 So please join us then at noon next week. 61:40 Please now join me in thanking members 61:43 of the Division of Perceptual Studies. 61:45 [APPLAUSE]